Imagination.
We tend to think of this as something special, an activity we engage in when, say, we're trying to do something creative like paint a picture, write a novel, or compose a song.
Or as what the brain does when it dreams -- which seemingly is markedly different from the clear perception of reality in our waking state.
But Evan Thompson's book, "Waking, Dreaming, Being," has given me a different perspective on imagination. From both a Buddhist and neuroscientific perspective, he sees imagination as permeating every form of consciousness.
Here's a passage from his Imagining: Are we real? chapter.
These ancient Indian and modern neuroscience views share a deep insight: sense impressions don't produce consciousness; they modulate it in the waking state. Sense impressions change the tone, pitch, or volume of consciousness, as do memories and emotions in the waking and dream states, but they don't create the vibration of consciousness itself.
We perceive the world thanks to our senses and our ability to move around, but sensing and moving don't fashion consciousness anew each time we wake up; they affect an already existing consciousness present also when we dream.
To get these ancient and modern ideas right, however, we need to understand dreaming as imagination, not false perception. Wakefulness isn't a hallucinatory state constrained by sensory inputs; it's an imaginative state fed by sense perception. Dreaming isn't a hallucinatory state cut off from sensory inputs; it's an imaginative state fed by memory and emotion.
As we've seen, imagination permeates waking and dreaming consciousness; in both states it underlies how we experience the ego or the "I-Me-Mine."
The religiously- and supernatural-minded shouldn't jump to the conclusion that Thompson believes there is any alternative to imagination being part and parcel of consciousness.
Meaning, he doesn't consider that consciousness is fundamental, immaterial, or capable of observing reality "as it is" (whatever that might mean). He writes:
The upshot is that there's no way to stand outside consciousness and look at it, in order to see how it fits into the rest of reality. Science always moves within the field of what consciousness reveals; it can enlarge this field and open up new vistas, but it can never get beyond the horizon set by consciousness. In this way, direct experience is primary and science secondary.
The primacy of direct experience requires that we reevaluate how we think about the project of relating consciousness and the brain. We can never step outside consciousness to see how it measures up to something else, and consciousness never appears or shows up apart from some context of embodiment.
...Consciousness is something we live, not something we have. We don't have consciousness in any sense that would allow us to lose it without ceasing to be what we are. Consciousness is our way of being, and it cannot be objectified, that is, treated as just another kind of object out there in the world, because it is that by which any object shows up for us at all.
Hard to argue with.
Science is a way of confirming direct experience, which on its own can be decidedly faulty, in the sense of mistakenly taking an act of imagination to reflect something that is actually there in an objective sense, observable and knowable by others.
Much of our everyday life, of course, is marked by states of consciousness which we don't usually take to be imaginative. Memories and anticipations, for example. We imagine what the past has been like, and what the future will be.
Neither of which reflects reality. (For example, here's a New York Times piece, "Why Our Memory Fails Us.")
So imagination is everpresent in human consciousness. It's just a matter of degree, and how it is expressed.
Fascinating about imagination and how science is learning so much more about the brain:
"Popular Science features new Dartmouth research that focuses on what the brain’s “mental workplace” looks like when people manipulate images in their mind.
“Our lab is very interested in the kind of flexible cognitive behaviors that humans have,” Alex Schlegel, a graduate student in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, and lead author of the study, tells Popular Science. “We can learn new things, we can think of new concepts, seeing things from different perspectives—a lot of this has to do with a very rich mental space, kind of a mental playground.”
Participants in the study were asked to look at pictures of abstract shapes, explains Popular Science. While undergoing an fMRI scan, they were asked to recall or manipulate the images in their mind. “We saw differences in activity all over the brain when we compared to control conditions,” Schlegel tells the magazine. “Rather than being a single area responsible for imagining or manipulating, it seems like lots of areas have to work in concert.”
Posted by: observer | December 19, 2014 at 02:45 PM
From both a Buddhist and neuroscientific perspective, he sees imagination as permeating every form of consciousness.
Also :
he doesn't consider that consciousness is fundamental, immaterial, or capable of observing reality "as it is" (whatever that might mean)
Why do people "consider" these things?
These are at best hypotheses, and not facts, and not proved/accepted theories either!
People can hypothesize anything under the sun (and investigate those hypothesizes), but why do they consider something is such and such (to themselves, and also publicly) without first coming to a proof of that position?
Theists do it with God. They "consider" God is a tablet-graffiti-artist, they "consider" God wants them to machine-gun schoolchildren, they "consider" all kinds of stuff.
And others "consider" imagination permeating every form of consciousness (when we aren't even quite agreed what consciousness is, or if animals have it, or if plants do), or that "there's no way to stand outside consciousness".
Why do people have such a hard time saying "I don't know"? They may well add : "I'm hypothesizing this, and am investigating (or would like to have investigated) this line of thinking", but that's a very different thing to say and think, isn't it?
Posted by: Appreciative Reader | December 21, 2014 at 05:35 AM
Why do people have such a hard time saying "I don't know"?
It's more difficult and less sociable to admit the extent of one's ignorance than it is to state something definitive, even if it is just speculation or suspicion. No one wants to be clueless, it seems.
Posted by: x | December 21, 2014 at 12:35 PM